The hidden logic of success - Part 2

THE HIDDEN LOGIC OF SUCCESS – PART 2

MARIO D’COUTO

Another aspect to the hidden logic of success is what psychologists call ‘chunking’. This is a word that has also been used by Roger Dawson, author of the book, “MAKE THE RIGHT DECISION EVERY TIME”. According to Dawson, chunking is connected with rapid reasoning. But what is rapid reasoning? To understand this, let us first understand what Alexander Hamilton has to say, who was one of the founding fathers of the United States of America. Whenever he faced a problem, this was how he would handle a problem as he says, “Men give me some credit of genius. All the genius I have lies in this: when I have a subject in hand, I study it profoundly. Day and night, it is before me. I explore it in all its bearings. My mind becomes pervaded with it. Then the efforts that I made are what people are pleased to call the fruits of genius. It is the fruit of labour and thought.”

Rapid reasoning is your ability to pull together unrelated facts from your pool of knowledge and focus them on the decision. The reason experts like Sir Isaac Newton have rapid reasoning powers is that they chunk information. Chunking describes what the mind does when it stores information in parcels rather than individual pieces. Remember when you first tied your shoelaces? You learned to pull both ends tight, cross them over into a simple bow, fold one loose end in half and wrap the other around it. Now, how many times do you think through all these steps when you put on a pair of sneakers? We no longer know what it takes to tie a shoe (because we know it in an instant) because we have chunked all those individual pieces of knowledge together.

People who have great intuition really become very adept at chunking information. It enables them to access huge amounts of information in seconds. The key to intuition, then, is saturating yourself with information about the decision and chunking that information to make rapid reasoning possible.

Consider another example. If I gave you a set of alphabets to remember by just looking at it for not more than 5 seconds and recall it without looking at the page, you probably would not be able to do that. For instance,

X  A  W M  Q  B  F  Z  Y  N  E  O  G

Did you try recalling it without looking at it? Be honest and ask yourself. On the other hand, if I gave you another set of alphabets,

A  B   N  O  R  M  A  L  I  T  I  E  S

I am sure this would not have taken you a second to remember. Why? Because you knew it all along. In 1973, William Chase and Herbert Simon, two American psychologists conducted an experiment. They had two groups of people, one who were actual chess masters and the other, a bunch of newbies in the game. The experiment consisted of remembering the different positions of the chess coins. So, the first time, all the chess coins were kept in the regular position after which both groups were asked to recall the positions of the coins after viewing for a few seconds. It was found that the chess masters were able to remember with ease while the amateurs struggled. Later, the position of the chess coins was shuffled and were kept in random positions. This time, both the groups, were once again asked to recall the positions of the chess coins. The amateurs, as expected, were not able to recall. But what was surprising was that the so-called chess masters experienced the same difficulty. This goes to show that talent is not something someone is born with. It is a process of repetitive action where the action becomes second nature.

When chess masters look at the positions of the pieces on a board, they see the equivalent of a word. Their long experience of playing chess enables them to ‘chunk’ the pattern with a limited number of visual fixations in the same way that our familiarity with language enables us to chunk the letters constituting a familiar word. It is a skill derived from years of familiarity with the relevant ‘language’, not talent. As soon as the language of chess is disrupted by the random positioning of pieces, chess masters find themselves looking at a jumble of letters, just like the rest of us.

The same principle can be applied to other sports or even other aspects of life. For instance, when Roger Federer returns a service, he is not demonstrating sharper reactions than you and I. Rather what he is showing is that he can extract more information from the service action of his opponent and other visual clues, enabling him to move into position earlier and more efficiently than the rest of us, which, in turn, allows him to make the return – in his case, a forehand cross – court winner.

The key point is that this is not something top sportsmen are born with. If you were to go back to the time when Roger Federer was learning technique, you would find that he was ponderous and sluggish. His movement would have been characterized by conscious control of the skill, lacking smoothness or unity. Only later, after countless hours of practice, were his skills integrated into an intricate set of procedures capable of flexible execution.

Today, Federer’s motor programme are so deeply ingrained that if you were to ask him how he is able to play an immaculately timed forehand, he wouldn’t be able to tell you. He might be able to talk about what he was thinking at the time or the strategic importance of the shot but he wouldn’t be able to provide any insight into the mechanics of the movements that made the stoke possible. Why? Because Federer has practiced for so long that the movement has been encoded in implicit rather than explicit memory. Psychologists call this the expert – induced amnesia.

The ascendancy of the mental and the acquired over the physical and the innate has been confirmed again and again, as Anders Ericsson puts it, “The most important differences are not at the lowest level of cells or muscle groups but at the athletes’ superior control over the integrated and co-ordinated actions of their bodies. Expert performance is mediated by acquired mental representations that allow the experts to anticipate, plan and reason alternative courses of action. These mental representations provide experts with increased control of the aspects that are relevant to generating their superior performance.” In other words, it is practice, not talent, that holds the key to success. Top performers are not born with sharper instincts, they possess enhanced awareness and anticipation.

Good decision-making is about compressing the informational load by decoding the meaning of patters derived from experience. This cannot be taught in a classroom; it is not something you are born with; it must be lived and learned. In other words, it emerges through practice.

Garry Kasparov, the famous chess player, was embedded in the living, breathing reality of playing chess. When he sees a chessboard, he does not chunk the pattern by relating it to an altogether different experience but by perceiving it immediately as the Sicilian Défense or the Latvian Gambit. His retrieval structure is rooted within the fabric of the game. This is the most powerful type of knowledge and is precisely the kind possessed by firefighters, top sportsmen and other experts. Kasparov himself said after winning game two of the 6 game match, “Had I been playing the same game against a very strong human, I would have had to settle for a draw. But I simply understood the essence of the end game in a way the computer did not. It’s computational power was not enough to overcome my experience and intuitive appreciation of where the pieces should go.”

Let us now look at some other examples,

a)      Art Fry thought of post – it – notes while singing in Church. His initial idea was for a bookmark that wouldn’t fall out, rather than a note on which to write. It was fortunate he worked for a company like 3M, which encourages its employees to follow up on their hunches. Lewis Lehr, the chairman of the company, says its corporate structure is ‘designed specifically to encourage young entrepreneurs to take an idea and run with it’.  He calls it the heart of their design of growth.
b)      Louis Pasteur, the French chemist and microbiologist, was examining some fermented grapes when he realized that the grapes ferment only when the skin is broken. He then knew that bacterial infection was caused by germs in the air, not by the spontaneous internal generation he previously thought responsible. His discovery saved the French beer, wine and silk industries and led to the process we now know as pasteurization.
c)      When Ray Kroc tried to buy the McDonald, brothers’ share of his company, they stunned him by asking for $2.7 million – an amount that would leave them a million dollars each after taxes. Kroc recalled, “I’m not a gambler and I did not have that kind of money but my funny bone instinct kept urging me on. So, I closed my office door, cussed up and down and threw things out of the window. Then I called my lawyer back and said, ‘Take it!’” It was a smart move, however much it hurt. Their share was soon worth $15 million a year to Kroc.
d)      In 1928, Alexander Fleming was about to throw away some bacteria he had been cultivating. A meld growth was contaminating the culture. But before disposing of the culture, he noticed a bacterium – free circle around the meld growth. A hunch led him to investigate further. He found a substance in the meld that prevented the growth of bacteria even when he diluted it 800 times. He called it penicillin.
e)      King Gillette was a salesman who sold cork that went inside bottle caps. It fascinated him. He would say, “Isn’t that something? I make my living selling something people throw away and keep on rebuying. I wonder what else would people buy, throw away and buy again?” That’s where he hit upon the idea of the disposable razor blade.

            All of these appear to be examples of a magical intuition when really, they are examples of rapid reasoning. Each of the people discussed above was an expert in his field. Their expertise had caused them to chunk their knowledge so they could access it easily. Ray Kroc had visited so many restaurants as a malted-machine-salesman that he could quickly analyse what the McDonald brothers had going for them. Louis Pasteur had spent 11 years studying fermentation before he tried it together in his mind with exposure to air. And King Gillette had spent his entire business life working with people who sold disposable items to the public.

            So how does one achieve this? There are 3 things,

a)      Accurately categorize the situation so you can start looking for the solution in the right direction.
b)      Blueprint the problem accurately, so your mind totally focuses on the problem it has to solve.
c)      Saturate your mind with facts about the problem. Remember, it is all about chunking information which is nothing but rapid reasoning.

            How long can it take? According to research, it could take a minimum of 10 years. In chess, for example, Herbert Simon and William Chase, two American psychologists, found that nobody had attained the level of an international grandmaster with less than a decade’s intense preparation with the game. In music composition, John Hayes, author of the book, “THE COMPLETE PROBLEM SOLVER”, states that it takes a good 10 years to achieve excellence.

            An analysis of the top 9 golfers of the 20th century shared that they won their first international competition at around 25 years of age, which was, an average, more than 10 years after they started golfing. The same finding has been discovered in fields as diverse as mathematics, tennis, swimming, long-distance running and other fields.

            Even among scholars, in a study of the 120 most important scientists and 123 most famous poets and authors of the 19th century, it was found that 10 years elapsed between their first work and their best work. Ten years, then, is the magic number for the attainment of excellence.

            At the beginning, the task of remembering thousands of words and fitting them together using abstract rules of grammar seems impossible. But after many years of experience, we can look at a random sentence and instantly comprehend its meaning. It is estimated that most English language users have a vocabulary 20,000 words. American psychologist Herbert Simon has estimated that chess masters command a comparable vocabulary pattern or chunks. This principle can of course be extended to other aspects of life. That’s when the knowledge becomes instinctual.




Popular posts from this blog

In the world yet not of the world

The Gift of Life

Are you creative?